In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
1
Improving the Quality of Parent-Child Contact in Separating Families with
Infants and Young Children: Empirical Research Foundations
Michael E. Lamb and Joan B. Kelly
Social scientists in general, and psychologists in particular, largely agree that
parent-child relationships play a crucial role in shaping children’s development and
adjustment. As a result, considerable efforts have been made to examine the
developmental course of these relationships, the features ensuring that some relationships
have more positive effects on children’s development than others, and the effects of
parent-child separations and relationship disruptions on children’s subsequent
adjustment. In this paper, we briefly summarize the relevant empirical literature, citing
the secondary rather than primary literature for the most part in order to minimize the
length and density of the text. We first discuss the development of infant- and child-
parent relationships, and the factors that affect the strength and quality of these
relationships. We then turn to research concerned with the effects of divorce on children,
with special focus on the ways in which harmful effects can often be minimized by
promoting the maintenance of children’s relationships with both of their divorcing
parents. We have discussed many of these issues in some detail elsewhere (.e.g., Kelly,
2005, 2007; Kelly & Emery, 2003; Kelly & Lamb, 2000, 2003; Lamb, 2002a, 2002b, in
press; Lamb & Lewis, 2005; Trinder & Lamb, 2005) but this chapter provides our most
up-to-date statement. Our goal is to provide those professionals conducting custody
evaluations or assessments with an underlying empirical foundation for understanding
and weighing the many complex issues seen in separating families, including those in
which parents are disputing custody and access arrangements. Knowledge of the social
science literature also provides evaluators and other professionals with a sound basis for
formulating and defending recommendations regarding children’s living arrangements to
the court if such recommendations are expected or requested (Kelly & Johnston, 2005).
THE DEVELOPMENT OF PARENT-CHILD RELATIONSHIPS
Kelly and Lamb (2000), among others, have documented how an understanding
of normative developmental phases informs both our understanding of the ways in which
parental separation and divorce may affect children’s development and adjustment as
well as the design of post-divorce living arrangements most likely to benefit children. As
described by Bowlby (1969), and largely confirmed by subsequent research (for detailed
review, see Thompson, 2006), Kelly and Lamb (2000) recounted how infant-parent
attachments pass through four broad developmental phases, during the first three of
which infants learn to discriminate among adult carers and gradually develop emotional
attachments to them.
In the first two months (phase 1), infants indiscriminately accept care from any
carer and use a repertoire of innate signals, including crying and smiling, to bring and
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
2
keep potential carers close to them. The relief of distress from hunger or pain and the
growing interest in and response to social signals from adults are the building blocks for
more discriminating attachment processes but regular interaction is needed to continue
the process of attachment formation because infants at this age have very primitive
memories and cognitive abilities.
In the period extending from 2 to 7 months of age (phase 2), infants increasingly
begin to recognize their parents and other carers and to prefer interaction with them. They
also begin to anticipate carers’ responses to their signals although they do not yet
understand that people (including carers) continue to exist when they are not present.
Infants of this age initiate and enjoy social interactions and start to show signs of
“attachment in the making.” They do not yet typically protest separations from their
parents, but require frequent contact with their parents for attachment formation to
continue. Importantly, research has shown that fathers are as competent to care for their
infants and toddlers as mothers, given comparable opportunities and experiences (Lamb,
1997, 2002a; Parke, 1996).
In the third phase of attachment development (between 7 and 24 months),
attachments become increasingly apparent, as infants preferentially seek to be near and to
interact with specific carers, by whom they are more easily soothed than by strangers.
Contrary to Bowlby’s initial speculation and widespread ‘common sense’, there is
considerable evidence that most infants in two parent families do not become attached to
their mothers first but rather form attachments to both parents at about the same age,
around 6 to 7 months (see Lamb, 2002a, for a review) even though fathers typically
spend less time with their infants than mothers do (Pleck & Masciadrelli, 2004). This
indicates that, although a threshold level of interaction is crucial for attachments to form,
time spent interacting is not the only critical dimension.
Infants begin to protest when separated from their primary attachment figures
around 6 to 7 months of age, react warily to strangers, and, in a rudimentary way, start
recognizing that parents continue to exist even when they are not present. Over the
ensuing months, infants and toddlers become able to tolerate longer separations from
their parents or attachment figures, although such separations may remain stressful. Most
infants come to ‘prefer’ the parents who take primary responsibility for their care
(typically their mothers), but this does not mean that relationships with their less-
involved parents are unimportant. In fact, many toddlers and preschoolers seem to
‘prefer’ their ‘traditional’ fathers, especially when they are not stressed or tired (Lamb,
2002a). The amounts of time that infants spend with their two parents does not appear to
determine whether or not the attachment relationships with either are insecure or secure.
However, the relative prominence of the two parents in caring for and interacting with
their children does appear to affect the relative importance of the two relationships with
respect to their impact on later development. Specifically, the relationship with the more
involved parent tends to have a greater impact than the relationship with the less involved
parent, albeit not clearly in direct proportion to the relative levels of involvement.
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
3
Nonetheless, both relationships remain psychologically important even when there are
disparities in the two parents’ levels of participation in child care.
According to attachment theorists, infants form attachments to those who have
been available regularly and have responded to the infants’ signals and needs (Lamb,
Thompson, Gardner, & Charnov, 1985; Thompson, 2006). All carers are not
equivalently sensitive, of course, and individual differences in responsiveness affect the
quality or security of the attachment relationships that form. The quality of both maternal
and paternal behavior is reliably associated with the security of infant-parent attachment
(DeWolff & Van IJzendoorn, 1997; Van IJzendoorn & DeWolff, 1997). Interestingly,
the association between the quality of paternal behavior and the quality of infant-father
attachment appears to be weaker than the parallel association between maternal behavior
and the security of infant-mother attachment. However, the quality of both mother- and
father-child interaction remains the most reliable predictor of individual differences in
psychological, social, and cognitive adjustment in infancy, as well as in later childhood
(Lamb & Lewis, 2005; Thompson, 2006).
According to Yarrow (1963; Yarrow & Goodwin, 1973), separation responses of
infants become increasingly intense as attachments to parents and other important carers
strengthen between 6 and 24 months. As mentioned earlier, most infants initially prefer
the parent who provides most of their care and are more likely to seek out their preferred
parents for comfort when distressed (see Lamb, 2002a, for a review). Non-preferred
parents remain emotionally important, however, and are sought out for other social and
emotional needs, and, when primary carers are not available, for comfort. Preference for
primary carers diminishes with age and often disappears by 18 months of age (Lamb,
2002a). Although infants and toddlers may resist transitions between parents in the
second year, just as they sometimes protest (even more strongly) daily transitions to out-
of-home care providers, they generally comfort quite quickly once the transition is
accomplished. This is particularly likely when both parents have the opportunity to
engage in normal parenting activities (feeding, playing, soothing, putting to bed, etc.)
while attachments are being established and consolidated.
Infants and toddlers need regular interaction with their “attachment figures” in
order to foster, maintain, and strengthen their relationships (Lamb, Bornstein, & Teti,
2002; Thompson, 2006). This means that young children need to interact with both
parents in a variety of contexts (feeding, playing, diapering, soothing, reading, putting to
bed, etc.) to ensure that the relationships are consolidated and strengthened. In the
absence of such opportunities for regular interaction across a broad range of contexts,
infant-parent relationships may weaken rather than grow stronger. When toddlers are
separated for as little as a few days from all of their attachment figures (for example, both
parents) simultaneously, intense distress and disturbances that persist for 6 months after
reunion have been reported (Bowlby, 1973; Heinicke, 1956; Heinicke & Westheimer,
1966; Robertson & Robertson, 1971). Reactions are muted, but not eliminated, when
children are cared for by other attachment figures or sensitive substitute carers during the
separation (Robertson & Robertson, 1971). Extended separations from parents with
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
4
whom children have formed meaningful attachments are thus undesirable because they
unduly stress developing attachment relationships (Bowlby, 1973). The loss or
attenuation of important attachment relationships may cause depression and anxiety,
particularly in the first two years, when children lack the cognitive and communication
skills that would enable them to cope with loss. The absence of regular contact slowly
erodes relationships, such that, over time, parents who do not interact regularly with their
infants effectively become strangers.
In the final phase of attachment formation, which begins around age two, toddlers
better understand why parents come and go, and can enter with their parents into some
joint planning around daily activities (Greenberg, Cicchetti, & Cummings, 1990;
Thompson, 2006). The increased cognitive and language abilities of 2- to 3-year-olds
enable them to tolerate somewhat longer separations from their parents without undue
stress. However, their very primitive sense of time prevents them from conceptualizing
much beyond today and tomorrow, inhibiting their ability to understand and cope with
lengthy separations of several weeks or months.
Relationships with parents continue to play a crucial role in shaping children’s
social, emotional, personal and cognitive development into middle childhood and
adolescence (Lamb & Lewis, 2005). Indeed, the quality of both mother- and father-child
relationships remains the most reliable correlates of individual differences in
psychological, social, and cognitive adjustment in infancy, as well as in later childhood
(Lamb & Lewis, 2005; Thompson, 2006). Not surprisingly, therefore, children in both
two- and single-parent families appear better adjusted when they enjoy warm positive
relationships with two actively involved parents (Amato & Gilbreth, 1999; Hetherington,
1999; Lamb, 1999, 2002b; Thompson & Laible, 1999). Children are better off with
insecure attachments than without attachment relationships, however, because these
enduring ties play essential formative roles in later social and emotional functioning.
There is also a substantial literature documenting the adverse effects of disrupted parent-
child relationships on children’s development and adjustment, with a linear relationship
between age of separation and later attachment quality in adolescents. The weakest
attachments to parents are reported by those whose parents separated in the first 5 years
of their lives (Woodward, Fergusson, & Belsky, 2000). Similarly, in a retrospective study
of adolescents whose parents had divorced, Schwartz and Finley (2005) found that age at
time of divorce was associated with ratings of both paternal involvement and nurturance,
indicating that the earlier the separation, the greater the impact on the quality of the
children’s relationships with their fathers.
THE EFFECTS OF PARENTAL SEPARATION OR DIVORCE
Researchers have clearly demonstrated that, on average, children benefit from
being raised in two biological or adoptive parent families rather than separated, divorced,
or never married single parent households (Amato, 2000; Aquilino, 1996; Carlson, 2006;
Clarke-Stewart & Brentano, 2006; Clarke-Stewart, Vandell, McCartney, Owen, & Booth,
2000; Hetherington, 1999; Hetherington & Kelly, 2002; McLanahan, 1999; McLanahan
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
5
& Sandefur, 1994; McLanahan & Teitler, 1999; Simons & Associates, 1996; Simons,
Lin, Gordon, Conger, & Lorenz, 1999), although there is considerable variability within
groups, and the differences between groups are relatively small. Indeed, although
children growing up in fatherless families are, on average, disadvantaged relative to peers
growing up in two-parent families with respect to psychosocial adjustment, behavior and
achievement at school, educational attainment, employment trajectories, income
generation, involvement in anti-social and even criminal behavior, and the ability to
establish and maintain intimate relationships, the majority of children with divorced
parents enjoy average or better-than-average social and emotional adjustment as young
adults (Booth & Amato, 2001; Clarke-Stewart & Brentano, 2006; Hetherington & Kelly,
2002; Kelly & Emery, 2003). Approximately 20-25% of children in post-separation and
divorced families give evidence of adjustment problems, compared to 12% in married
families. Thus, the majority of children from separated families evince no
psychopathology or behavioral symptoms, although they are likely to experience psychic
pain for at least some period of time (Emery, 1998; Hetherington & Kelly, 2002;
Laumann-Billings & Emery, 2000; Schwartz & Finley, in press).
Such individual differences in outcomes force us to identify more precisely both
the ways in which divorce/single parenthood may affect children’s lives and the factors
that might account for individual differences in children’s adjustment following their
parents’ separation. Five inter-related factors appear to be especially significant.
Economic Stresses
Typically, single parenthood is associated with a variety of social and financial
stresses with which custodial parents must cope, largely on their own. Single parent
families are more economically stressed than two parent families, and economic stresses
or poverty appear to account (statistically speaking) for many effects of single parenthood
(Hetherington & Kelly, 2002; McLanahan, 1999).
Reductions in Time with Children
Because single mothers need to work more extensively outside the home than
married or partnered mothers do, parents spend less time with children in single-parent
families and the levels of supervision and guidance are lower and less reliable than in
two-parent families (Hetherington & Kelly, 2002; McLanahan, 1999). Reductions in the
level and quality of parental stimulation and attention may affect achievement,
compliance, and social skills while diminished supervision makes antisocial behavior and
misbehavior more likely (Hetherington & Kelly, 2002).
Conflict Between Parents
Conflict between the parents commonly precedes, emerges or increases during the
separation and divorce processes, and often continues for some time beyond them.
Interparental conflict is an important correlate of children’s psychosocial adjustment just
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
6
as marital harmony, its conceptual inverse, appears to be a reliable correlate of positive
adjustment (Cummings et al., 2004; Johnston, 1994; Kelly, 2000). The negative impacts
of high levels of marital conflict on the quality of parenting of both mothers and fathers
have been well documented. In general, parental conflict is associated with more
rejecting, less warm and nurturing parenting by mothers, and with fathers’ withdrawal
from parenting and engagement in more intrusive interactions with their children
(Cummings & Davies, 1994; Grych, 2005). Anger-based marital conflict is associated
with filial aggression and externalizing behavior problems, perhaps because such parents
and children have similar difficulty regulating negative affect (Katz & Gottman, 1993).
These and other data support the observation that some of the ‘effects of divorce’ are
better viewed as the effects of pre-separation marital conflict and violence (Kelly, 2000).
The adversarial legal system tends to promote conflict between already vulnerable
parents because of its win-lose orientation and the way it fosters hostile behaviors and
demands. Although the adversarial process purports to focus on children’s “best
interests”, parents’ psychologically-driven legal strategies more often represent their
own needs and perceived entitlements, and the effect is to diminish the possibility of
future civility, productive communication, and cooperation (Kelly, 2003).
Quality and Type of Parenting
The quality and type of parenting have emerged as important influences on the
post-separation/divorce adjustment of school-aged children and adolescents.
Deterioration in the quality of parenting after separation has long been recognized
(Belsky, Youngblood, Rovine, & Volling, 1991; Clarke-Stewart & Brentano, 2006;
Hetherington, 1999; Sturge-Apple, Davies, & Commings, 2006; Wallerstein & Kelly,
1980). Many parents are preoccupied, stressed, emotionally labile, angry, and depressed
following separation, and their “diminished parenting” includes less positive involvement
and affection expressed with their children, and more coercive and harsh forms of
discipline. Additional internal factors affecting quality of parenting include parents’
psychological adjustment, violence, and high conflict. External factors such as
absorption in dating, new partners, cohabitation, remarriage, and poverty and financial
instability are also associated with reductions in the quality of parenting (Amato, 2000;
Hetherington, 1999; Kelly, 2000; Pruett, Williams, Insabella, & Little, 2003; Simons,
Lin, Gordon, et al, 1999; Wallerstein & Kelly, 1980).
Researchers have identified specific aspects of parenting that can moderate the
impact of separation and divorce on children’s social, emotional, and academic
adjustment, and potentially protect children against the harmful impacts of high conflict.
Effective parenting by separated mothers is characterized by warmth, authoritative
discipline (setting limits, noncoercive discipline and control, enforcement of rules,
appropriate expectations), academic skill encouragement, and monitoring of the
children’s activities (Amato & Fowler, 2002; Buchanan, 1996; Hetherington, 1999;
Martinez & Forgatch, 2002; Simons et al, 1999). As described in more detail below, the
effective parenting of fathers that is linked to more positive adjustment following divorce
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
7
is also associated with effective paternal behaviours including active involvement (help
with homework and projects, emotional support and warmth, talking about problems and
involvement in school (Amato & Fowler, 2002; Amato & Gilbreth, 1999; Hetherington,
1999; Finley & Schwartz, in press).
Disruptions in Relationships with Fathers
Divorce commonly disrupts one of the child’s most important and enduring
relationships, that with his or her father. As Amato (1993; Amato & Gilbreth, 1999) has
shown with particular clarity, however, the bivariate associations between father absence
and children’s adjustment are much weaker than one might expect. Indeed, Amato and
Gilbreth’s (1999) meta-analysis revealed no significant association between the
frequency of father-child contact and child outcomes, largely because of the great
diversity in the types of ‘father-present’ relationships. We might predict that contacts
with abusive, incompetent, or disinterested fathers are likely to have much different
effects than relationships with devoted, committed, and sensitive fathers. As expected,
Amato and Gilbreth (1999) found that children’s well-being was significantly enhanced
when their relationships with nonresident fathers were positive, when the nonresident
fathers engaged in “active parenting,” and when contact was frequent. Dunn, Cheng,
O’Connor, and Bridges (2004), Simons and Associates (1996), Hetherington, Bridges,
and Insabella (1998), and Clarke-Stewart and Hayward (1996) likewise reported that
children benefited when their nonresident fathers were actively involved in routine
everyday activities, and the conclusion was clearly supported in recent analyses by
Carlson (2006) of data from the National Longitudinal Study of Youth. Carlson showed
that father involvement was associated with better adolescent adjustment and that
paternal involvement partially mediated the effects of family structure (notably divorce or
single parenthood) on adolescents’ behavioral outcomes. Similarly, higher levels of
paternal involvement in their children’s schools was associated with better grades, better
adjustment, fewer suspensions, and lower dropout rates than were lower levels of
involvement (Nord, Brimhall, & West, 1997). Active engagement in a variety of specific
activities and on-going school-related discussions between fathers and their adolescents
significantly lowered the probably of school failure when compared to adolescents with
less actively engaged fathers.
Another meta-analysis indicated that, on multiple measures of emotional and
behavioral adjustment and academic achievement by mothers, fathers, teachers, and
clinicians, children in joint physical custody were better adjusted than children in sole
custody arrangements. In fact, children in shared custody were as well adjusted as
children whose parents remained married (Bauserman, 2002). Although joint physical
custody parents reported less past and current conflict than did sole physical custody
parents, conflict did not explain the superiority of the children in joint custody
arrangements. Again, the clear implication is that active paternal involvement, not simply
the number or length of meetings between fathers and children, predicts child adjustment.
This suggests that post-divorce arrangements should specifically seek to maximize
positive and meaningful paternal involvement rather than simply allow minimal levels of
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
8
visitation. As in non-divorced families, in other words, the quality of continued
relationships with the parents--both parents--is crucial (Kelly & Lamb, 2000). Stated
differently and succinctly, the better (richer, deeper, and more secure) the parent-child
relationships, the better the children’s adjustment, whether or not the parents live together
(Lamb, 2002a, 2002b).
A recent longitudinal study of representative samples of adolescents living in low
income neighbourhoods in Boston, San Antonio, and Chicago nicely illustrated the
associations over time between non-resident paternal involvement and adolescent
delinquency after statistically controlling for the effects of influences such as
demographic factors and the quality of mother-child relationships (Coley & Medeiros,
2007). As expected, non-resident paternal involvement was associated with less
delinquency overall; importantly, higher paternal involvement was associated with
declines in delinquency over time, particularly among adolescents who were more
involved with delinquent activities. In addition, as delinquency increased, paternal
involvement increased too, suggesting that fathers were responding to changes in their
children’s problem behaviour. Similarly, in another longitudinal study of adolescents,
Menning (2006) showed that adolescents whose non-resident fathers were more involved
were less likely to start smoking.
Given the demonstrated importance of fathers’ active participation and effective
parenting, the influence of maternal attitudes on the extent of paternal involvement in the
marriage and following separation and divorce is important (Cowdery & Knudson-
Martin, 2005; Pleck, 1997). Mothers can be influential gatekeepers of paternal
involvement through attitudes and behaviors that limit or facilitate fathers’ opportunities
to parent and develop close relationships with their children. Mothers’ traditional
attitudes toward women’s roles, identities linked primarily to caregiving, and perceptions
that mothers are more competent at child care than fathers are associated with more
active inhibitory gate-keeping, particularly following separation, and this is linked with
lower levels of father involvement (Allen & Hawkins, 1999; Fagan & Barnett, 2003).
Overall, then, a number of factors help account for individual differences in the
effects of divorce, and because they are inter-correlated, it is difficult to assess their
relative importance. The ability to maintain meaningful relationships with both parents
does appear to be of central importance, however, both in its own right and as a correlate
of some of the other factors. As shown later, thoughtful interventions can take advantage
of these inter-correlations, initiating processes that minimize the adverse effects on
children’s adjustment by striving to promote healthy relationships between children and
both of their parents, whether or not they live together.
MINIMIZING THE ADVERSE EFFECTS OF DIVORCE
Even though children’s best interests are usually served by keeping both parents
actively involved in their children’s lives, many custody and access arrangements may
not foster the maintenance of relationships between children and their non-resident
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
9
parents. The use of traditional “visiting guidelines” in many jurisdictions which prescribe
every other weekend to the nonresident parent (with perhaps a brief midweek visit), or
the reliance of mental health professionals on unsubstantiated beliefs that every other
weekend is the best parenting plan for children, has caused great dissatisfaction and sense
of loss among the majority of children in post-divorce arrangements. Research on
children’s and young adults’ views of their post-divorce living arrangements indicates
that the majority express strong wishes and longing for more time with their fathers, a
desire for more closeness, and favorable views of shared physical custody as their
preferred schedule (Fabricius & Hall, 2000; Laumann-Billings & Emery, 2000; Smith &
Gallop, 2001). Arbitrary and one-size-fits-all time restrictions on the amounts of time
children spend with their fathers have been the norm regardless of the quality of the
father-child relationship. It is no less important to maintain both child-parent attachments
when the divorced parents had ‘traditional’ roles before divorce than when they shared
parenting responsibilities more equitably. Our focus should remain on the children’s best
interests, not “fairness” to the parents.
Writing on behalf of 18 experts on the effects of divorce and contrasting
parenting plans, Lamb, Sternberg, and Thompson (1997, p. 400) observed more than a
decade ago that: “To maintain high-quality relationships with their children, parents need
to have sufficiently extensive and regular interactions with them, but the amount of time
involved is usually less important than the quality of the interaction that it fosters. Time
distribution arrangements that ensure the involvement of both parents in important
aspects of their children’s everyday lives and routines...are likely to keep nonresidential
parents playing psychologically important and central roles in the lives of their children.”
In order for parents to have a positive impact on their children’s development, it is
important that parents be integral parts of their children’s lives. This remains especially
important as children get older and greater portions of their time are occupied outside the
family by virtue of friendships, extracurricular activities, sports, and the like. At all ages,
it is important for parents to know teachers and friends, what’s happening at school or
pre-school, how relationships with peers are going, what other activities are important or
meaningful to the children, etc., and to be aware of daily ups-and-downs in their
children’s lives. It is hard to do this without regular and extensive first hand involvement
with their children in a variety of contexts.
As Kelly and Lamb (2000; Lamb & Kelly, 2001; Lamb, 2002b) reiterated, the
ideal situation is one in which children with separated parents have opportunities to
interact with both parents frequently in a variety of functional contexts (feeding, play,
discipline, basic care, limit-setting, putting to bed, etc.). The evening and overnight
periods (like extended days with naptimes) with nonresidential parents are especially
important psychologically for infants, toddlers and young children. They provide
opportunities for crucial social interactions and nurturing activities, including bathing,
soothing hurts and anxieties, bedtime rituals, comforting in the middle of the night, and
the reassurance and security of snuggling in the morning that 1 to 3 hour long visits
cannot provide. According to attachment theory, as noted earlier, these everyday
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
10
activities promote and maintain trust and confidence in the parents, while deepening and
strengthening child-parent attachments, and thus need to be encouraged when decisions
about access and contact are made.
One implication is that even young children should spend overnight periods with
both parents when both have been involved in their care prior to separation, even though
neo-analysts have long counseled against this (Kelly & Lamb, 2000; Lamb & Kelly,
2001). As Warshak (2000) has pointed out, the prohibition of overnight “visitation” has
been justified by prejudices and beliefs rather than by any empirical evidence. When both
parents have established significant attachments and both have been actively involved in
the child’s care, overnight ‘visits’ will consolidate attachments and child adjustment, not
work against them. Consistent with this reasoning, the results of recent research by Pruett
and her colleagues (2005) show that regular overnight visits were associated with better
adjustment on the part of toddlers and young children. Parents who have been actively
involved before divorce but are then denied overnight access to their children are thereby
excluded from an important array of activities, and the strength or depth of their
relationships suffer as a result.
Solomon and Biringen (2001) challenged Kelly and Lamb’s (2000) conclusions
regarding the beneficial effects of overnight visits for many young children, citing the
results of a study by Solomon and George (1999a, 1999b). Contrary to their assertions,
however, these researchers did not find that overnight visits with non-custodial fathers
adversely affected the security of infant-mother attachment, or that overnights were more
problematic for preschoolers than for infants and preschoolers. In addition, many of the
infants and toddlers they studied had never lived with their two parents and may thus not
have formed attachments to their fathers before the overnight visits commenced; their
situation is much different than that of infants and toddlers who have established
attachments to two involved parents prior to separation/divorce which was the context
explicitly addressed by Kelly and Lamb (2000). Different steps are needed when
promoting the formation rather than the maintenance of attachments, as Kelly and Lamb
(2003) pointed out in another paper concerned with young children whose parents live
too far apart for children to have regular contact with both of them. Solomon and George
(1999a, p. 27) also noted that some of the infants in their study had experienced extended
and repeated separations from their fathers, which, as noted above, would have stressed
these relationships further.
To minimize the deleterious impact of extended separations from either parent,
furthermore, attachment theory tells us there should be more frequent transitions than
would perhaps be desirable with older children (Kelly & Lamb, 2000). To be responsive
to young children’s psychological needs, in other words, the parenting schedules adopted
for children under age two or three should actually involve more transitions, rather than
fewer, to ensure the continuity of both relationships and to promote the children’s
security and comfort. Although no empirical research exists testing specific parenting
plans following separation, it is likely, for example, that infants and toddlers would
remain most comfortable and secure with a schedule that allowed the child to see his or
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
11
her father at least three times a week, including at least one overnight extended stay
(assuming father is adequate), so that there is no separation of greater than 2-3 days.
From the third year of life, the ability to tolerate longer separations begins to increase, so
that most toddlers can manage two consecutive overnights with each parent without
stress.
Interestingly, psychologists have long recognized the need to minimize the length
of separations from attachment figures when devising parenting plans, but they have
typically focused only on separations from mothers, thereby revealing their presumption
that young children are not meaningfully attached to their fathers, or that paternal
involvement is a peripheral influence. The lingering resistance to overnights among
professionals working with divorcing families appears to view fathers as essential
strangers to their infants and toddlers, rather than as important attachment figures. To the
extent that children are attached to both of their parents, however, separations from both
parents are stressful and at minimum generate psychic pain. As a result, parenting plans
that allow children—especially very young children--to see their fathers every other
Saturday for a few hours, or every other shortened weekend (with perhaps a brief mid-
week visit) clearly fail to recognize the adverse consequences of weeklong separations
from non-resident parents. It is little wonder that such arrangements lead to attenuation
of the relationships between non-resident parents and their children. Instead, it is
desirable to promote continued involvement by both parents, striving when necessary to
increase the participation of those parents (typically fathers) whose limited prior
involvement may initially make overnight contact inappropriate (Kelly & Lamb, 2003).
Recently, Fabricius and Luecken (2007) found that the more time a group of university
students had lived as children with their fathers the better were their relationships with
their fathers, independent of amount of parent conflict. More time with fathers appeared
to be beneficial in both high and low conflict families, and more exposure to parent
conflict appeared to be detrimental (poorer health status and more distress) at both high
and low levels of amount of time with fathers. Inter-parental conflict should thus be
avoided wherever possible, but its presence should not be used to justify restrictions on
children's access to either of their parents.
RESTRICTING CONTACT IN RESPONSE TO MARITAL CONFLICT
Of course, there are some cases in which the possible benefits of keeping both
parents involved are outweighed by the costs of doing so. Conflict-filled or violent
relationships between the parents are most likely to trigger such cost-benefit analyses
because, as noted earlier, continuing high conflict is reliably associated with poorer child
outcomes following divorce (Johnston, 1994; Kelly, 2000; Maccoby & Mnookin, 1992).
Children in high conflict families who have frequent contact with their fathers are more
poorly adjusted than those in low conflict families (Amato & Rezac, 1994; Hetherington,
1999; Johnston, 1994; Johnston, Kline, & Tschann, 1989; Maccoby & Mnookin, 1992).
Inter-parental conflict should thus be avoided wherever possible, but litigation-related
conflict and conflict triggered by the high levels of stress around the time of divorce do
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
12
not appear to have enduring consequences for children. As a result, their occurrence
should not be used to justify restrictions on children’s access to either of their parents.
Maccoby and Mnookin (1992) cautioned that minor or isolated instances of
domestic violence should not affect decisions regarding custody and visitation. The high
conflict found harmful by researchers such as Johnston (1994; Johnston & Roseby, 1997)
typically involved repeated incidents of spousal violence and verbal aggression between
parents with substantial psychiatric problems and personality disorders that continued
after divorce at intense levels for extended periods of time, often in front of the children.
As a result, Johnston has emphasized the importance of continued relationships with both
parents except in those relatively uncommon circumstances in which intense, protracted
conflict or violence occurs and persists. According to Maccoby and Mnookin (1992),
somewhere around a quarter of divorcing families experience high levels of conflict
around the time of divorce, and perhaps 10% of them may have conflict that is
sufficiently severe and intractable that it may not be beneficial for the children concerned
to have contact with their non-resident parents (see also Johnston, 1994). Not all
entrenched post-divorce conflict involves both parents’ participation, however, because
in a significant number of cases, one parent has essentially disengaged emotionally from
the other parent, and is not promoting or instigating the conflict, but is a victim of the
other parent’s rage and vindictiveness, intransigence, and/or failure to comply with
parenting and financial orders, and therefore may need to return to court for assistance
(Friedman, 2004; Kelly, 2003, 2005).
Significant numbers of children have warm and supportive relationships with
parents who have highly conflicted and/or violent relationships with one another, so we
must be careful when reports of parental conflict are allowed to influence decisions about
parent-child contact (Holden, Geffner, & Jouriles, 1998; Maccoby & Mnookin, 1992;
Sternberg & Lamb, 1999). According to Appel and Holden (1998), 60% of the children
whose parents were violent with one another were not themselves victims of physical
child abuse, suggesting that decision-makers need to assess the relationships with parents
directly and not simply assume that children must have been abused because their parents
were violent with one another. Unfortunately, however, mere allegations of conflict or
even marital violence can be powerful tools in an adversarial system, frequently resulting
in reduced levels of court-approved contacts between fathers and children (Sternberg,
1997). Disagreements about the occurrence, nature, and perpetrators of violence are
quite common, furthermore, and do not always reveal self-serving biases (Braver &
O’Connell, 1998; Sternberg, Lamb, & Dawud-Noursi, 1998).
Newer research which differentiates among types of intimate partner violence is
directly relevant to these issues of custody and access. Types of partner violence
identified include Situational Couple Violence, instigated by both men and women in
fairly equal numbers, characterized most often by more minor forms of violence
(pushing, shoving, grabbing). Injuries are not as common, and fear of the partner is not
typical. Situational couple violence typically arises from arguments that spiral upward
and poor conflict management skills, rather than power, coercion and control as central
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
13
dynamics (Babcock, Costa, Green, & Eckhardt, 2004; Johnson, 2005; Johnson & Leone,
2005; Johnston & Campbell, 1993; Kelly & Johnson, in press; Stets & Straus, 1992). In
contrast, Battering (also called Intimate Terrorism), seen primarily in shelter and hospital
samples, is primarily male perpetrated, accounts for most of the injuries seen in women,
and has coercion, control, and emotional abuse as the primary dynamics. Separation
Instigated Violence has also been identified in couples whose prior history together did
not include any violence or coercion and control (Johnston & Campbell, 1993; Kelly,
1982). Although the experience of parental violence is always distressing, if not
traumatic, for children, these differences among types of partner violence have
implications for various post-separation interventions, screening, and the development of
appropriate parenting plans for children (Jaffe, Johnston, Crooks, & Bala, in press; Kelly
& Johnson, in press).
Research on the impact of post- as opposed to pre-divorce conflict on children’s
adjustment has yielded mixed results. Some investigators have found that marital conflict
is a more potent predictor of post-divorce adjustment than post-divorce conflict (Booth &
Amato, 2001; Buehler, Krishnakumar, Stone, Anthony, Pemberton, Gerard, & Barber,
1998; King & Heard, 1999; Kline, Johnston, & Tschann, 1991), whereas Hetherington
(1999) found that post-divorce conflict had more adverse effects than did conflict in
married families. Booth and Amato (2001) reported no association between the amount
of post-divorce conflict and later adjustment in young adults. The varied findings may
reflect the use of different measures, a failure to differentiate between types of conflict
after divorce, differing parental styles of resolution, and/or variations in the extent to
which children are directly exposed to anger and conflict. High conflict is more likely to
be destructive post-divorce when parents use their children to express their anger, and are
verbally and physically aggressive on the phone or in person. By contrast, when parents
continue to have conflict, but encapsulate it and do not put their children in the middle,
children appear unaffected (Buchanan et al., 1991; Hetherington, 1999). Buffers have
also been identified which may protect children from the harmful effects of high parent
conflict, including a good relationship with at least one parent or caregiver, parental
warmth, and emotional support from a sibling (Vandewater & Lansford, 1998;
Hetherington & Kelly, 2002).
In addition, most experts agree that conflict localized around the time of
separation and divorce is of less concern than conflict that was and remains an intrinsic
and unresolved part of the parents’ relationship and continues after their divorce
(Cummings et al., 2004; Cummings & O’Reilly, 1997; Johnston & Roseby, 1997).
Similarly, conflict from which children are shielded also does not appear to affect
adjustment (Hetherington, 1999) whereas conflict that includes physical violence is more
pathogenic than high conflict without violence (Jouriles, Norwood, McDonald, Vincent,
& Mahoney, 1996; McNeal & Amato, 1998).
The quality of the relationships between non-residential parents and their children
is also crucial when determining whether to sever or promote relationships between
divorced parents and their children. Regardless of the levels of violence, there are many
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
14
families in which non-resident fathers and children have sufficiently poor relationships--
perhaps because of the fathers’ psychopathology, substance or alcohol abuse – that
“maintenance” of interaction or involvement may not be of net benefit to the children, but
we do not know how many relationships are like this. Unrepresentative data sets, such as
those collected by Greif (1997) in the course of research designed to study fathers and
mothers who lose contact with their children after divorce, suggest that perhaps ten to
fifteen percent of parents do not have either the commitment or individual capacities to
establish and maintain supportive and enriching relationships with their children
following divorce. Taken together, Johnston’s and Greif’s estimates suggest that, at
most, 15% to 20% (depending on how greatly the two groups of parents overlap) of the
children whose parents divorce might not benefit from regular and extended contact with
their non-resident parents. Stated differently, of course, this suggests that more than
three-quarters of the children experiencing their parents’ divorce could benefit from
having and maintaining relationships with their non-resident parents. Instead of
‘standard’ parenting plans, therefore, individual circumstances should be examined to
ensure that the arrangements made are sensitive to the parents’ and children’s strengths,
schedules, and needs (Kelly, 2005, 2007; Smythe & Chisholm, 2006).
INTERVENTIONS WHICH PROMOTE CHILDREN’S RESILIENCY
In response to empirical research demonstrating the factors associated with risk in
children whose parents have separated, a number of different interventions and best
practices have been developed that focus on parents’ conflict and communication, quality
of parenting, children’s reactions to separation and divorce, development of appropriate
parenting plans, and sustainable agreements. Among the hierarchy of interventions
available in the court system and community, both public and private, are education
programs for separating parents, custody and divorce mediation programs, judicial
settlement conferences, collaborative lawyering, specialized programs for parents with
continuing high conflict post-divorce, and Parenting Coordination. While many programs
have face validity, and generally receive high ratings on parent satisfaction and self-
reports of reduced parent conflict, only a small percentage have been empirically
evaluated (Kelly, 2002; Trinder & Lamb, 2005).
The most widespread and inexpensive interventions are education programs for
separating parents which explain to parents the effects of divorce on children, the impact
of parent conflict, the particular risk when parents use their children to express their
anger and disagreement, the need to separate children’s needs from adult needs,
parenting skills, and often provide skill-based training to minimize conflict and promote
more effective communication. At least in the short term, these courses appear to be
effective (e.g., Arbuthnot & Gordon, 1996; Bacon & McKenzie, 2004; Ellis & Anderson,
2003; Pedro-Carroll, Nakhnikian, & Montes, 2001), particularly when the content is
empirically-based and includes skill-based training and role-play experiences. More
extensive research-based parent education programs appear to bring about meaningful
behavioural changes in both mothers and fathers (for review, see Haine, Sandler,
Wolchik et al., 2003; Braver, Griffin, Cookson et al., 200?).
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
15
Of all the divorce-related interventions implemented to date, custody and divorce
mediation enjoys the most empirical support regarding the benefits to divorcing and
divorced families (Emery, 1994; Emery, Laumann-Billings, Waldron, et al, 2001; Kelly,
1996, 2002, 2004). The potential benefits are substantial in both the short term (e.g.,
earlier settlement of parenting disputes, reduced parental conflict, improved parental
support), and the longer term (e.g., more sustained contact between fathers and children
12 years later), relative to parents using the traditional adversarial system (Emery et al,
2001; Kelly, 2004).
The increasing use of Parenting Coordinator over the past two decades in the
United States represents an effort to address the needs of children in situations of very
high conflict. Parenting Coordination is a child-focused dispute resolution process
designed to assist parents with a continuing history of high conflict to settle disputes
regarding their children in a timely manner, and to facilitate compliance with parenting
plans and related court orders. A non-adversarial intervention, the Parenting
Coordination role combines case and conflict management, relevant parent education,
mediation, and, when specified, arbitration of certain child-related disputes which the
parents cannot settle on their own. Parenting Coordination is generally viewed as a post-
decree process for parents who have demonstrated a continuing inability to settle child-
specific disputes through other means, including private negotiations, mediation,
specialized parent education groups, settlement conferences, or following trial. Most
Parenting Coordinators serve by stipulation of the parents and court order, or by private
consent agreements. In general, the judiciary has been supportive and enthusiastic; other
professionals involved with these families have also indicated in surveys that parent
coordinators can be very effective, especially in reducing the frequency and intensity of
disputes (Coates et al., 2004). Parenting Coordinators in jurisdictions with the most
experience have revised and refined the role more precisely, which is reflected in the
Guidelines for Parenting Coordination developed by an AFCC task force (2006), and the
need for specialized training for mental health and legal professionals undertaking this
role has been further articulated (Guidelines, 2006; Kelly, in press). While there is as yet
no systematic research on the long-term effectiveness, dramatic decreases in the rates of
litigation (court appearances) in the first year after Parent Coordinators were appointed
was reported in one unpublished dissertation (Johnson, 1994). Several articles in a recent
special issue of Family Courts Review (2001, Volume 39, issue 3) on the most
problematic post divorce families make clear that parent coordinators are among the most
important interventions available to smooth the post-separation and divorce transitions,
and this point was underscored in the recent article by Coates and her colleagues (2004).
There are a number of best practices that mental health professionals can provide
to help separating parents reduce the risk that their children potentially face. First, they
can prepare parents to talk to their children about those aspects of the separation and
divorce that directly affect their children. Most children are not informed about the
details and meaning of the parental separation for them, are not invited to ask questions
nor allowed an opportunity to give suggestions regarding their new living arrangements,
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
16
and not consulted regarding schedule changes (Dunn, Davies, O’Connor, & Sturgess,
2001; Smart, 2002; Smart & Neale, 2000), which leaves children to cope with these
major changes in their lives without either emotional support or cognitive clarity.
Secondly, professionals can become familiar with and promote awareness of
various models for parenting plans which have been developed partly in response to
research such as that described in this chapter. Instead of one standard visiting pattern, a
menu of different time-sharing options for children of different ages encourages parents,
professionals, and courts to consider children’s ages and developmental needs and
achievements; the quality of parent-child relationships, parents’ interest and capacity to
be involved in their children’s lives, and children’s voices and input when appropriate.
Parenting plan models typically include multiple options for various living arrangements
that range from traditional visiting, expanded access involving more midweek and
weekend time, and variations of shared physical custody arrangements. Many of the
parenting plan models available include highly detailed templates to guide parental
choices with respect to legal decision-making and parental responsibilities (see Note 1 for
examples). It is important that agreements and court orders are sufficiently detailed so
that both parents know when, where, and how their children will make the transitions
between households, eliminating the ambiguity that angry parents exploit and which
often cause one or both parents to argue in front of their children. Orders that use the
vague language of ‘liberal visitation’ invite sustained argument, and, when mothers
remain angry, do not sufficiently protect children’s relationships with their fathers. Even
in legal cultures that emphasize ‘private ordering,’ lawyers, therapists, and custody
evaluators should help parents develop specific schedules for children’s time with each
parents, and courts should routinely expect such language and detail.
Thirdly, because transitions between households provide opportunities for
discussion and argument, it is important when parents have a history of conflict to ensure
that these exchanges take place in neutral settings and at times that limit contact between
the parents. Parenting plans that extend weekends to Monday morning and use mid-week
overnights (assuming adequate parenting skills) rather than brief 3 hour visits have been
used extensively by mediators, lawyers, and Parenting Coordinators in the past decade in
the United States. In such plans, one parent drops the children off at school, and the other
parent picks them up at the end of the school day or from day care. Parenting
Coordinators report that many children benefit from the elimination of face-to-face parent
confrontations at the family residence or other meeting places, and conflict becomes a
much less frequent experience. Increasing number of locations now have visitation
exchange centers where children can be brought by one parent to reunite with the other
without making it necessary for the parents to encounter one another directly. Such
arrangements may be especially helpful when either the parents or children are fearful,
and have been shown to increase the frequency of contact between non-resident parents
and their children while reducing levels of conflict between the parents (Flory, Dunn,
Berg-Weger, & Milstead, 2001).
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
17
Fourth, custody evaluators should become familiar and stay current with the
empirical literature regarding attachment, child development, parent-child relationships,
separation and divorce and children’s adjustment. If they are to make recommendations
to the court, a subject currently generating some controversy (Tippins & Wittman, 2005),
these recommendations should be grounded in and supported by the current research
literature, rather than theory or subjective bias (Kelly & Johnston, 2005).
CONCLUSION
In all, basic research on early social development and descriptive research on the
multifaceted correlates of divorce have together yielded a clearer understanding of the
ways in which divorce affects children and of how the welfare of many children could be
enhanced by changes in common practices. Most importantly, we know that children
benefit from supportive relationships with both of their parents, whether or not those
parents live together. We also know that relationships are dynamic and are thus
dependent on continued opportunities for interaction. In order to ensure that both adults
become or remain parents to their children, post-divorce parenting plans need to
encourage participation by both parents in as broad as possible an array of social contexts
on a regular basis. Brief dinners and occasional weekend visits do not provide a broad
enough or extensive enough basis for such relationships to be fostered, whereas weekday
and weekend daytime and nighttime activities are important for children of all ages. In
the absence of sufficiently broad and extensive interactions, many fathers drift out of
their children’s lives, and children see their fathers as increasingly peripheral, placing
these children at risk psychologically and materially. It is not clear exactly how much
time is necessary to ensure that both parents stay involved in their children’s lives, and
undoubtedly this will vary from family to family and the age of the children. Braver and
O’Connell (1998) have suggested that at least one-third of the non-school hours should
be spent with the non-resident parent and most experts would agree that 15% (every other
weekend) is almost certainly insufficient.
To date, unfortunately, policy makers, judges, and lawyers in many jurisdictions
have not been very attentive to the importance of promoting children’s relationships with
both of their parents. Representative statistics are not readily available (see Smyth,
2004), although there is general satisfaction in many countries and jurisdictions (and
among mothers) with post-separation arrangements that ensure ‘regular contact’ without
sufficient effort made to ensure that the contact is sufficient for children to be able to
maintain meaningful relationships with their non-resident parents, who are of course
overwhelmingly fathers. The default arrangements recommended by most lawyers or
solicitors typically involve ‘every other weekend’ visits, rather than arrangements that
allow non resident fathers to really be involved in their children’s lives. Furthermore,
reasoned discussion about the potential benefits of greater involvement by non resident
parents has been drowned out by poisonous rhetoric from groups and individuals mired in
gendered concerns about fairness for parents rather than children’s best interests. On the
other hand, as we have made clear in the previous section, many innovative practices and
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
18
programmes have been developed over the last few decades as increasing numbers of
professionals have come to recognise the importance of promoting continued
relationships between children and both of their separating parents. These trends augur
well for the future.
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
19
Note 1
For examples of parenting plan models, see Alaska Court System, Model Parenting
Agreement, www.state.ak.us/courts/forms/dr-475.pdf; American Academy of
Matrimonial Lawyers, Model for a Parenting Plan; State Bar of Arizona, Model Parenting
Time Plans—for Parent/Child Access,
http://www.supreme.state.az.us/dr/Pdf/Parenting_Time_Plan_Final.pdf; Association of
Family and Conciliation Courts, Planning for Shared Parenting, www.afccnet.org;
[email protected] ( for research annotated schedules.)
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
20
REFERENCES
Allen, S .M. & Hawkins, A. J. (1999). Maternal gatekeeping: Mothers’ beliefs and
behaviors that inhibit greater father involvement in family work. Journal of
Marriage & Family, 61, 199-212.
Amato, P. R. (1993). Children’s adjustment to divorce: Theories, hypotheses, and
empirical support. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, 23-38.
Amato, P. R. (2000). The consequences of divorce for adults and children. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 62, 1269-1287.
Amato, P. R. & Fowler, F. (2002). Parenting practices, child adjustment, and
family diversity. Journal of Marriage and Family 64, 703-716.
Amato, P. R., & Gilbreth, J.G. (1999). Non-resident fathers and children's well-being: A
meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 557-573.
Amato, P.R., & Rezac, S. (1994). Contact with residential parents, inter-parental conflict,
and children’s behavior. Journal of Family Issues, 15, 191-207.
Appel, A. E., & Holden, G. W. (1998). The co-occurrence of spouse and physical child
abuse: A review and appraisal. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 578-599.
Aquilino, W. S. (1996). The life course of children born to unmarried mothers:
Childhood living arrangements and young adult outcomes. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 58, 293-310.
Arbuthnot, J., & Gordon, D.A. (1996). Does mandatory divorce education for parents
work? A six-month outcome evaluation. Family & Conciliation Courts Review,
34, 60-81.
Babcock, J. C., Costa, D. M., Green, C.E., & Eckhardt, C.I. (2004). What situations
induce intimate partner violence? A reliability and validity study of the Proximal
Antecedents to Violent Episodes (PAVE) Scale. Journal of Family Psychology,
18, 433-442.
Bacon, B. L. & McKenzie, B. (2004). Parent education after separation/divorce: Impact
of the level of parental conflict on outcomes. Family Court Review, 42, 85-98.
Baris, M. A., Coates, C. A., Duvall, B. B., Johnson, L. T., & La Cross, E. R. (2001).
Working with high conflict families of divorce: A guide for professionals. New
York: Jason Aronson.
Bauserman, R.. (2002). Child adjustment in joint-custody versus sole-custody
arrangements: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 91-102.
Belsky, J., Youngblade, L., Rovine, M. & Volling, B. (1991). Patterns of marital change
and parent-child interaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 487-498.
Booth, A., & Amato, P.R. (2001). Parental predivorce relations and offspring postdivorce
well-being. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 197-212.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New
York: Basic Books.
Braver, S. L., & O’Connell, E. (1998). Divorced dads: Shattering the myths. New York:
Tarcher, Putnam.
Buchanan, C.M, Maccoby, E.E., & Dornbusch, S.M. (1991). Caught between parents:
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
21
Adolescents’ experience in divorced homes. Child Development, 62, 1008-1029.
Buehler, C., Krishnakumar, A., Stone, G., Anthong, C., Pemberton, S., Gerard, J. &
Barber, B. (1998). Interparental conflict styles and youth problem behaviors: A
two-sample replication study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 119-134.
Carlson, M. J. (2006). Family structure, father involvement, and adolescent behavioural
outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 137-154.
Clarke-Stewart, A. & Brentano, C. (2006). Divorce: Causes and consequences. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., & Hayward, C. (1996). Advantages of father custody and contact
for the psychological well-being of school-age children. Journal of Applied
Developmental Psychology, 17, 239-270.
Clarke-Stewart, K.A., Vandell, D.L., McCartney, K., Owen, M.T., & Booth, C. (2000).
Effects of parental separation and divorce on very young children. Journal of
Family Psychology, 13, 304-326.
Coates, C. A., Deutsch, R., Starnes, H., Sullivan, M. J., & Sydlik, B. (2004). Parenting
coordination for high-conflict families. Family Court Review, 42, 246- .
Coley, R. L., & Medeiros, B. L. (2007). Reciprocal longitudinal relations between
nonresident father involvement and adolescent delinquency. Child Development,
78, 132-147.
Cowdery, R.S. & Knudson-Martin, C. (2005). The construction of motherhood: Tasks,
relational connection, and gender equality. Family Relations, 54 (3), 335-345.
Cummings, E. M., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Raymond, J. (2004). Fathers in family
context: Effects of marital quality and marital conflict. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The
role of the father in child development (Fourth edition; pp. 196-221). Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley.
Cummings, E. M., & O’Reilly, A. W. (1997). Fathers in family context: Effects of
marital quality on child adjustment. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father
in child development (Third edition, pp. 49-65). New York: Wiley.
DeWolff, M. S., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-
analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68,
571-591.
Dunn, J., Davies, L., O’Connor, T., & Sturgess, W. (2001). Family lives and friendships:
The perspectives of children in step-, single-parent, and two-parent families.
Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 272-287.
Dunn, J., Cheng, H., O’Connor, T.G. & Bridges, L. (2004). Children’s perspectives on
their relationships with their nonresident fathers: influences, outcomes and
implications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 553-566.
Ellis, D. & Anderson, D. Y. (2003). The impact of participation in a parent education
program for divorcing parents on the use of court resources: An evaluation study.
Conflict Resolution Quarterly, 169-187.
Emery, R. E. (1998). Marriage, divorce, and children’s adjustment. (2nd Edition).
Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Fabricius, W.V. & Hall, J. (2000). Young adults’ perspectives on divorce: Living
arrangements. Family & Conciliation Courts Review, 38, 446-461.
Fagan, J., & Barnett, M. (2003). The relationship between maternal gatekeeping, paternal
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
22
competence, mothers’ attitudes about the father role, and father involvement.
Journal of Family Issues, 24, 1020-1043.
Finley, G. E., & Schwartz, S. J. (In press). Father involvement and young adult outcomes:
The differential contributions of divorce and gender. Family Court Review.
Flory, B. E., Dunn, J., Berg-Weger, M., & Milstead, M. (2001). An exploratory study of
supervised access and custody exchange services. Family Court Review, 39, 469-
Friedman, M. (2004). The so-called high-conflict couple: A closer look. American
Journal of Family Therapy, 32, 101-117.
Greenberg, M. T., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (Eds.) (1990). Attachment in the
preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Greif, G. (1997). Out of touch: when parents and children lose contact after divorce.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Grych, J. H. (2005). Interparental conflict as a risk factor for child maladjustment:
Implications for the development of prevention programs. Family Court Review,
43, 97-108.
Guidelines for Parenting Coordination (2006). AFCC Task Force on Parenting
Coordination. Family Court Review, 44(1), 164-181.
Haine, R.A., Sandler, I. N., Wolchik, S. A., Tein, J-U., & Dawson-McClure, S. R.
(2003). Changing the legacy of divorce: Evidence from prevention programs and
future directions. Family Relations, 52, 397-405.
Heinicke, C. (1956). Some effects of separating two-year-old children from their parents:
A comparative study. Human Relations, 9, 105-176.
Heinicke, C., & Westheimer, I. (1966). Brief separations. New York: International
Universities Press.
Hetherington, E.M. (Ed.) (1999). Should we stay together for the sake of the children?
In E.M. Hetherington (Ed.), Coping with divorce, single parenting, and
remarriage (pp. 93-116). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hetherington, E. M., Bridges, M., & Insabella, G. M. (1998). What matters? What does
not? Five perspectives on the association between marital transitions and
children's adjustment. American Psychologist, 53, 167-184.
Hetherington, E.M., & Kelly, J. (2002). For better or for worse: Divorce reconsidered.
New York: Norton.
Holden, G. W., Geffner, R., & Jouriles E. W. (Eds.) (1998). Children exposed to family
violence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Jaffe, P. G., Johnston, J. R., Crooks, C. V., & Bala, N. (in press). Custody disputes
involving allegations of domestic violence: The need for differentiated
approaches to parenting plans. Family Court Review.
Johnson, M. P. (2005). Apples and organs in child custody disputes: Intimate terrorism
vs. situational couple violence. Journal of Child Custody, 2, 43-52.
Johnson, M. P. & Leone, J. M. (2005). The differential effects of intimate terrorism and
situational couple violence: Findings from the National violence against women
survey. Journal of Family Issues, 26, 322-349.
Johnson, T. (1994). Analysis of Santa Clara County, CA case files. Unpublished
dissertation.
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
23
Johnston, J. R. (1994). High-conflict divorce. The Future of Children, 4, 165-182.
Johnston, J.R. & Campbell, L. (1993). A clinical typology of interparental violence in
disputed-custody divorces. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 63, 190-199.
Johnston, J. R., Kline, M., & Tschann, J. (1989). Ongoing post-divorce conflict in
families contesting custody: Effects on children of joint custody and frequent
access. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 576-592.
Johnston, J.R., & Roseby, V. (1997). In the name of the child: A developmental
approach to understanding and helping children of conflict and violent
divorce. New York: Free Press.
Jouriles, E. N., McDonald, R., Norwood, W., Vincent, J. P., & Mahoney, A. (1996).
Physical violence and other forms of interpersonal aggression: Links with
children’s behavior problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 223-234.
Katz, L.F., & Gottman, J.M. (1993). Patterns of marital conflict predict children’s
internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 29, 940-
950.
Kelly, J. B. (1982). Divorce: The adult experience. In B. Wolman and G. Stricker
(Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
Kelly, J. B. (2000). Children’s adjustment in conflicted marriage and divorce: A decade
review of research. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 39,
963-973.
Kelly, J. (2002). Psychological and legal interventions for parents and children in custody
and access disputes: Current research and practice. Virginia Journal of Social
Policy and Law, 10, 129-163.
Kelly, J. (2003). Parents with enduring child disputes: Multiple pathways to enduring
disputes. Journal of Family Studies, 9, 37-50. (Australia)
Kelly, J. B. (2004). Family mediation research: Is there support for the field? Conflict
Resolution Quarterly, 22, 3-35.
Kelly, J. B. (2005). Developing beneficial parenting plan models for children following
separation and divorce. Journal of American Academy of Matrimonial Lawyers,
19, 237-254.
Kelly, J. B. (2007). Children’s living arrangements following separation and divorce:
Insights from empirical and clinical research. Family Process, 46, 35-52.
Kelly, J. B. (2008). Training for the parenting coordinator role. Journal of Child
Custody.
Kelly, J. B., & Emery, R.E. (2003). Children’s adjustment following divorce: Risk and
resilience perspectives. Family Relations, 52, 352-362.
Kelly, J. B. & Johnson, M. P. (2008). Differentiation among types of domestic
violence: Research updates and implications for interventions. Family Court
Review.
Kelly, J. B. & Johnston, J. R. (2005). Commentary on Tippins and Whitmann’s
“Empirical and ethical problems with custody recommendations: A call for
clinical humility and judicial vigilance”. Family Court Review, 4, 233-241.
Kelly, J. B., & Lamb, M. E. (2000). Using child development research to make
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
24
appropriate custody and access decisions for young children. Family and
Conciliation Courts Review, 38, 297-311.
Kelly, J. B., & Lamb, M. E. (2003). Developmental issues in relocation cases involving
young children: when, whether, and how? Journal of Family Research, 17, 193-
205.
King, V., & Heard, H.E. (1999). Nonresident father visitation, parental conflict, and
mother’s satisfaction: What’s best for child well-being? Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 61, 385-396.
Kline, M., Johnston, J., & Tschann, J. (1990). The long shadow of marital conflict:
A model of children’s postdivorce adjustment. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 53, 297-309.
Lamb, M. E. (1997). The development of father-infant relationships. In M. E. Lamb
(Ed.), The role of the father in child development (Third edition; pp. 104-120;
332-342). New York: Wiley.
Lamb, M. E. (1999). Non-custodial fathers and their impact on the children of divorce.
In R. A. Thompson & P.R. Amato (Eds.), The post-divorce family: Research and
policy issues (pp. 105-125). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lamb, M. E. (2002a). Infant-father attachments and their impact on child
development. In C. S. Tamis-LeMonda & N. Cabrera (Eds.), Handbook of father
involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 93-117). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Lamb, M. E. (2002b). Placing children’s interests first: Developmentally appropriate
parenting plans. The Virginia Journal of Social Policy and the Law, 10, 98-119.
Lamb, M. E. (in press). Improving the quality of parent-child contact in separating
families. In M. Maclean (Ed.), Contact between children and separated parents.
Oxford: Hart Publishing.
Lamb, M. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Teti, D. M. (2002). Development in infancy (Fourth
Edition). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lamb, M E., & Kelly, J. B. (2001). Using the empirical literature to guide the
development of parenting plans for young children: A rejoinder to Solomon and
Biringen. Family Courts Review, 39, 365-371.
Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2004). The development and significance of father-child
relationships in two-parent families. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father
in child development (Fourth edition; pp. 272-306). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley &
Sons, 2004.
Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2005). The role of parent-child relationships in child
development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science:
An advanced textbook (Fifth edition; pp. 429 - 468). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, 2005.
Lamb, M. E., Sternberg, K. J., & Thompson, R. A. (1997). The effects of divorce and
custody arrangements on children’s behavior, development, and adjustment.
Family and Conciliation Courts Review, 35, 393-404.
Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W. P., & Charnov, E. L. (1985). Infant-mother
attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Laumann-Billings, L, & Emery, RE. (2000). Distress among young adults in divorced
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
25
families. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 671-687.
Maccoby, E. E., & Mnookin, R. H. (1992). Dividing the child: Social and legal
dilemmas of custody. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
McLanahan, S.S. (1999). Father absence and the welfare of children. In E.M.
Hetherington (Ed.), Coping with divorce, single parenting, and remarriage (pp.
117-146). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McLanahan, S. S., & Sandefur, G. (1994). Growing up with a single parent: What hurts,
what helps. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
McLanahan, S. S., & Teitler, J. (1999). The consequences of father absence. In M.E.
Lamb (Ed.), Parenting and child development in “nontraditional” families. (pp.
83-102). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McNeal, C., & Amato, P. R. (1998). Parents' marital violence: Long-term consequences
for children. Journal of Family Issues, 19, 123-139.
Menning, C. L. (2006). Nonresident fathers’ involvement and adolescents’ smoking.
Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, 47, 32-46.
Nord, C., Brimhall, D., & West, J. (1997). Fathers’ involvement in their children’s
schools. National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education,
Washington, D.C. 20208-5574.
Parke, R. (1996). Fatherhood. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Pedro-Carroll, J., Nakhnikian, E., & Montes, G. (2001). Assisting children through
transition: Helping parents protect their children from the toxic effects of ongoing
conflict in the aftermath of divorce. Family Court Review, 39, 377-
Pleck, J. H., & Masciadrelli, B. (2004). Paternal involvement: Levels, sources, and
consequences. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development
(Fourth edition, pp. 222-271). New York: Wiley.
Pruett, M. K., Williams, T. Y., Insabella, G., & Little, T. D. (2003). Family and legal
indicators of child adjustment to divorce among families with young children.
Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 169-180.
Pruett, M. K., Insabella, G. M., & Gustafson, K. (2005). The Collaborative Divorce
Project: A court-based intervention for separating parents with young children.
Family Courts Review, 43, 38-51.
Robertson, J., & Robertson, J. (1971). Young children in brief separation: A fresh look.
Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 26, 264-315.
Schwartz, S. J., & Finley, G. E. (2005). Divorce-related variables as predictors of young
adults’ retrospective fathering reports. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 44,
145-163.
Simons, R.L. and Associates (1996). Understanding differences between divorced and
intact families. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Simons, R.L., Lin, K.H., Gordon, L.C., Conger, R.D., & Lorenz, F.O. (1999). Explaining
the higher incidence of adjustment problems among children of divorce compared
with those in two-parent families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 1020-
1033.
Smart, C. (2002). From children’s shoes to children’s voices. Family Court Review, 40,
307-319.
Smart, C. & Neale, B. (2000). “It’s my life too” – Children’s perspectives on post-
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
26
divorce parenting. Family Law, March, 163-169. (England)
Smith, A.B., & Gallop, M. M. (2001). What children think separating parents should
know. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 30, 23-31.
Smyth, B. M. (2004). Parent-child contact schedules after divorce. Family Matters, 69,
32-43. (Australia Institute of Family Studies, Australia)
Smyth, B. M., & Chisholm, R. (2006). Exploring options for parental care of children
following separation: A primer for family law specialists. Australian Journal of
Family Law, 20, 193-218.
Solomon, J., & Biringen, Z. (2001). Another look at the developmental research:
Commentary on Kelly and Lamb’s “Using child development research to make
appropriate custody and access decisions for young children.” Family Courts
Review, 39, 355-364.
Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999a). The development of attachment in separated and
divorced families: Effects of overnight visitation, parent, and couple variables.
Attachment and Human Development, 1, 2-33.
Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999b). The effects of overnight visitation in divorced and
separated families: A longitudinal follow-up. In J. Solomon & C. George (Eds.),
Attachment disorganization (pp. 243-264). New York: Guilford.
Sternberg, K. J. (1997). Fathers, the missing parents in research on family violence. In
M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (Third edition, pp.
284-308; 392-397). New York: Wiley.
Sternberg, K. J., & Lamb, M. E. (1999). Violent families. In M. E. Lamb (Ed), Parenting
and child development in “nontraditional” families (pp. 305-325). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Sternberg, K. J., Lamb, M. E., & Dawud-Noursi, S. (1998). Understanding domestic
violence and its effects: Making sense of divergent reports and perspectives. In
G. W. Holden, R. Geffner, & E. W. Jouriles (Eds.), Children exposed to family
violence (pp. 121-156). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Stets, J. & Straus, M. (1992). The marriage license as a hitting license. Physical violence
in American families. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (2006). Hostility and withdrawal
in marital conflict: Effects on parental emotional unavailability and inconsistent
discipline. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 227-238.
Thompson, R. A. (2006). Early sociopersonality development. In W. Damon, R. A.
Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child development, Volume 3.
Social, emotional, and personality development (Sixth Edition). Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley.
Thompson, R. A., & Laible, D.J. (1999). Noncustodial parents. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.),
Parenting and child development in “nontraditional” families (pp. 103-123).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tippins, T.M. & Wittmann, J. P. (2005). Empirical and ethical problems with custody
recommendations: A call for clinical humility and judicial vigilance. Family
Court Review, 43,
Trinder, L. & Lamb, M. E. (2005). Measuring up? The relationship between correlates of
children’s adjustment and both family law and policy in England. Louisiana Law
In R. M. Galazter-Levy, J. Kraus, & J. Galatzer-Levy. (2009). The scientific basis of child custody
decisions. (Second edition). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. (pp. 187-214)
27
Review, 65, 1509-1537.
Van lJzendoorn, M. H., & DeWolff, M. S. (1997). In search of the absent father—Meta-
analyses of infant-father attachment: A rejoinder to our discussants. Child
Development, 68, 604-609.
Vandewater, E. & Lansford, J. (1998). Influences of family structure and parental
conflict on children’s well-being. Family Relations, 47, 323-330.
Wallerstein, J. & Kelly, J. (1980). Surviving the breakup: How children and parents cope
with divorce. NY: Basic Books.
Warshak, R.A. (2000). Blanket restrictions: Overnight contact between parents and
young children. Family and Conciliation Courts Review, 38, 422-445.
Woodward, L., Ferguson, D.M., & Belsky, J. (2000). Timing of parental separation and
attachment to parents in adolescence: Results of a prospective study from birth to
age 16. Journal of Marriage & Family, 62, 162-174.
Yarrow, L. J. (1963). Research in dimensions of early maternal care. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 9, 101-114.
Yarrow, L. J., & Goodwin, M. (1973). The immediate impact of separation: Reactions of
infants to a change in mother figures. In L. J. Stone, H. T. Smith, & L. B.
Murphy (Eds.) The competent infant (pp. 1032-1040). New York: Basic Books.